(CP-16) Meaning
based knowledge
Meaning-Based Knowledge: Understanding the Psychology of Semantic Memory: General to Specific
Abstract: Semantic memory is our knowledge of the world, concepts, and their relationships, acquired through understanding their meaning, which is organized into categories and networks of related concepts. Meaning-based knowledge is a crucial aspect of semantic memory, structured hierarchically from broad categories to specific concepts. It enables us to make predictions, problem-solve, and make decisions. Prototype theory explains how we form mental representations of concepts based on typical examples, while semantic networks interconnect concepts, making them accessible and constantly updating with new information. Meaning-based knowledge has implications for language processing, education, and artificial intelligence. Teachers can activate relevant concepts to help students acquire and organize knowledge. Incorporating meaning-based knowledge in machine learning algorithms can improve machine language understanding. A deeper understanding of meaning-based knowledge can aid in cognitive psychology studies on perception, memory, language, and problem-solving.
Introduction: As a Psychology student, you may be familiar with the concept of semantic memory - the part of our long-term memory that stores our knowledge about the world, concepts, and relationships among them. But have you ever wondered how we acquire and organize this knowledge? The answer lies in the concept of meaning-based knowledge. In this article, we'll explore what meaning-based knowledge is, how it is formed and structured, and its significance in our cognitive processes.
Meaning-Based Knowledge
Meaning-based knowledge is the knowledge we acquire through understanding the meaning of a word or concept. Unlike episodic memory (memory of events), semantic memory is not based on personal experiences but rather on our knowledge of the world. Meaning-based knowledge refers to the organization of our semantic memory into categories and networks of related concepts.
Formation of Meaning-Based
Knowledge:
Meaning-based
knowledge is formed through our experiences and
interactions with the environment. When we encounter a new word or
concept, we use our
prior knowledge to understand its meaning. For example, if we
encounter the word "dog," we may already have a mental representation
of what a dog is, based on our previous experiences with dogs. We may know that
dogs are animals that bark, have fur, and are often kept as pets.
Once we have acquired a basic understanding of a concept, we continue to refine and expand our knowledge through further experiences and learning. For instance, we may learn more specific information about dogs, such as their breeds, characteristics, and behaviors. This new information is then integrated into our existing knowledge network of dogs.
How is Meaning-Based Knowledge
Structured?
Meaning-based knowledge is structured in a hierarchical manner, with general categories at the top and more specific concepts at the bottom. At the highest level, we have general categories, such as animals or vehicles. These categories are very broad and cover a wide range of concepts. As we move down the hierarchy, we meet more specific categories, such as mammals or cars. Finally, at the end of the hierarchy, we have individual concepts, such as dogs or Ferraris.
Significance of Meaning-Based
Knowledge:
Meaning-based knowledge is important to our cognitive processes, including perception, comprehension, and communication. Our ability to understand and communicate effectively relies on our ability to use meaning-based knowledge. When we encounter a new concept or word, we use our existing knowledge to understand its meaning. Our knowledge of categories and semantic relationships allows us to make inferences and predictions about the world around us.
Role in problem-solving and decision-making: Meaning-based knowledge also plays a crucial role in problem-solving and decision-making. When we are faced with a problem, we draw upon our semantic memory to generate possible solutions. Our ability to retrieve and apply relevant knowledge depends on the organization and accessibility of our meaning-based knowledge.
Key features:
Prototype Theory:
One important feature of meaning-based knowledge is prototype theory. Prototype theory suggests that we form mental representations of concepts based on a typical or ideal example of that concept. For instance, when we think of the concept of bird, we may form a mental image of a sparrow or a robin - a typical example of a bird. However, not all birds fit this prototype, and our mental representation may need to be adjusted to include other types of birds.
Semantic Networks:
Another important feature of meaning-based knowledge is semantic networks. Semantic networks refer to the way our semantic memory is organized into a network of interconnected concepts. When we encounter a new concept, we activate related concepts in our semantic network, allowing us to make connections and conclusions. For example, when we hear the word "cake," we may activate concepts such as "dessert," "baking," and "sugar."
Semantic networks are not fixed or static, but rather are constantly changing and updating. Our experiences and learning can modify the connections between concepts and change the strength of those connections. For example, if we learn a new fact about dogs, such as the fact that some breeds are hypoallergenic, this may change our understanding of the concept of dog and its relationship to other concepts.
Applications of Meaning-Based Knowledge:
Our understanding of meaning-based knowledge has important implications for a variety of fields, including language processing, education, and artificial intelligence.
Language Processing: Meaning-based knowledge is essential for language processing. When we hear or read a sentence, we use our knowledge of word meanings and relationships to understand its meaning. For example, in the sentence "The cat chased the mouse," we use our knowledge of the concepts of "cat" and "mouse" and their relationship to understand the action being described.
Education: An understanding of meaning-based knowledge can also be applied to education. Teachers can help students acquire and organize new knowledge by activating relevant concepts in their semantic network. By connecting new information to existing knowledge, students are more likely to remember and apply what they have learned.
Artificial Intelligence: Finally, meaning-based knowledge has implications for the development of artificial intelligence. One challenge in creating intelligent machines is giving them the ability to understand and use language. By incorporating knowledge of meaning-based knowledge into machine learning algorithms, researchers can help machines better understand the meaning of words and sentences.
Conclusion
In conclusion, meaning-based knowledge is a fundamental aspect of our cognitive processes. Our ability to acquire, organize, and use our knowledge of the world relies on the organization and accessibility of our semantic memory. By understanding the features of meaning-based knowledge, we gain insight into how we think and process information, and how we can improve our cognitive abilities.
References:
- Barsalou, L. W.
(1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and brain sciences, 22(4),
577-660.
- Barsalou, L. W.
(2008). Grounded cognition. Annual review of psychology, 59, 617-645.
- Barsalou, L. W.
(2016). On staying grounded and avoiding Quixotic dead ends. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1122-1142.
- Barsalou, L. W.,
Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding
conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 7(2), 84-91.
- Borghi, A. M.,
& Cimatti, F. (2010). Embodied cognition and beyond: Acting and
sensing the body. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 763-773.
- Clark, A.
(2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future
of cognitive science. Behavioral and brain sciences, 36(3), 181-204.
- Fuster, J. M. (2003). Cortex and mind: unifying cognition. Oxford University Press.
- Gick, M. L.,
& Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive
psychology, 12(3), 306-355.
- Hofstadter, D. R. (2001). Epilogue: Analogy as the core of cognition. In The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science (pp. 499-538). MIT Press.
- Kousta, S. T.,
Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The
representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 14-34.
- Lakoff, G.
(1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about
the mind. University of Chicago Press.
- Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. Basic books.
- Martin, A.
(2007). The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annual Review
of Psychology, 58, 25-45.
- Miller, G. A.
(1991). The science of words. Scientific American, 265(3), 86-92.
- Murphy, G. L.
(2002). The big book of concepts. MIT press.
- Paivio, A.
(1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian
journal of psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, 45(3), 255-287.
- Paivio, A.,
Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and
meaningfulness values for 925
No comments:
Post a Comment