Followers

Monday, April 17, 2023

Cognitive Psychology: Perception of Movement; Understanding the Mechanisms

 (CP-14) Perception of Movement: Understanding the Mechanisms



Abstract:

This article provides an in-depth understanding of the perception of movement, including the cognitive and neural mechanisms involved. Movement perception enables us to interact with our environment and engage in various activities. There are different types of movement perception, such as apparent, induced, autokinetic, short-range, and long-range movement. The mechanism of movement perception involves the detection, integration, and interpretation of motion signals, supported by various brain areas such as V1, MT, and MST. Understanding these mechanisms can improve training programs and safer driving strategies. The article emphasizes the importance of the perception of movement in our daily lives, enabling us to detect and interpret changes in the position of an object. Perception of movement has significant implications and is a complex and dynamic process involving several stages of visual processing and neural mechanisms in the brain.

Introduction:Top of Form

IntroductionfffffBottom of FormTop of Form

The perception of movement is an essential aspect of our daily experience, allowing us to navigate through our environment and interact with the world around us. This article will provide an in-depth understanding of the cognitive and neural mechanisms involved in the perception of movement, including how the movement is perceived, the brain areas involved, and the different types of movement perception.

What is the Perception of Movement?

The perception of movement refers to the ability to detect and interpret changes in the position of an object over time. Our brains process visual input from our environment, allowing us to perceive motion accurately, which is crucial for tasks such as catching a ball, driving, and crossing a busy street.

Definition of Movement Perception:

Movement perception is the process of detecting and interpreting motion in the environment. It involves extracting information about the speed, direction, and trajectory of moving objects and integrating this information to form a coherent representation of movement.

What the Perception of Movement Implies:

The perception of movement has significant implications for our daily lives. It allows us to interact with the world around us, navigate through our environment, and engage in various activities such as sports and driving.

How the Movement is Perceived:

There are several ways in which movement is perceived, including apparent movement, induced movement, autokinetic movement, and short-range and long-range movement.

Perception of Apparent Movement:

Apparent movement is the perception of movement that is not physically present in the environment. It occurs when two or more stationary images are presented in quick succession, creating the illusion of motion.

Sequence of Successive Images that Create Apparent Movement:

Apparent movement occurs when there is a sequence of successive images presented in quick succession. Each image is presented for a short period, creating the illusion of motion when viewed together.

Characteristics of the Apparent Movement:

The characteristics of the apparent movement include the speed, direction, and trajectory of the perceived motion. These characteristics can be manipulated by altering the interval between the successive images, the duration of each image, and the position of the images.

·         Short-range Movement: Short-range movement refers to the perception of motion that occurs within a limited visual field. It is characterized by the movement of objects that are within a few degrees of each other and can be easily tracked by the eyes.

·         Long-range Movement: Long-range movement refers to the perception of motion that occurs over a greater distance, typically outside the range of visual tracking. It involves the integration of motion signals across a larger visual field.

Perception of Induced Movement:

Induced movement is the perception of motion that is created by a moving background or context. For example, when watching a movie, we perceive the movement of characters and objects on the screen, even though the screen itself is stationary.

Autokinetic Movement:

Autokinetic movement is the perception of motion that occurs when a stationary object appears to move in the absence of external movement. It is believed to occur due to the natural movements of the eyes, which create small involuntary movements that can be perceived as motion.

Mechanism of Movement Perception:

The mechanism of movement perception involves several stages, including motion detection, motion integration, and motion interpretation.

Phases of Movement Perception:

The first phase of movement perception involves the detection of local motion signals in the visual input. These signals are then integrated across space and time to form coherent motion patterns. Finally, the brain interprets the motion signals to create a representation of movement.

Perception of Movement and the Brain:

The perception of movement is supported by different areas of the brain, including the primary visual cortex (V1), the middle temporal area (MT), and the medial superior temporal area (MST). These areas are involved in the processing of motion signals and the interpretation of movement.

Motion Detection:

The detection of motion signals involves the activity of specialized neurons known as direction-selective cells, which respond selectively to motion in specific directions. These cells are located in the middle temporal area (MT) and medial superior temporal area (MST) and are thought to integrate motion signals from different parts of the visual field to detect movement accurately.

Conclusion:

In conclusion, the perception of movement is a complex and dynamic process that involves several stages of visual processing and neural mechanisms in the brain. The perception of movement has significant implications for our daily lives, and understanding its cognitive and neural mechanisms can inform the development of better training programs for athletes and safer driving strategies for drivers. Further research in this area can shed light on the mechanisms underlying perception and improve our understanding of the visual system's capabilities.

References:

  1. Blake, R., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 13-21.
  2. Britten, K. H., & Newsome, W. T. (1998). Motion processing and sensory-motor integration. In Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 11, pp. 221-244). Elsevier.
  3. Culham, J. C., & Kanwisher, N. G. (2001). Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Current opinion in neurobiology, 11(2), 157-163.
  4. Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Hawken, M. J. (2016). Perception of motion. In The New Visual Neurosciences (pp. 685-696). MIT Press.
  5. Huk, A. C., & Shadlen, M. N. (2005). Neural activity in macaque parietal cortex reflects temporal integration of visual motion signals during perceptual decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(45), 10420-10436.
  6. Kourtzi, Z., & DiCarlo, J. J. (2006). Learning and neural plasticity in visual object recognition. Current opinion in neurobiology, 16(2), 152-158.
  7. Lisberger, S. G., & Movshon, J. A. (2015). Visual motion perception. Annual review of neuroscience, 38, 369-385.
  8. Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T., & Oeltermann, A. (2001). Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature, 412(6843), 150-157.
  9. Maunsell, J. H., & Newsome, W. T. (1987). Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Annual review of neuroscience, 10(1), 363-401.
  10. Mather, G., & Moulden, B. (1980). A simultaneous shift in apparent direction: further evidence for a “distribution shift” model of direction coding. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(3), 325-333.
  11. McIntosh, R. D., & Lashley, G. (2008). The neurobiology of visual attention: from neurons to cognition. Progress in Neurobiology, 86(4), 345-372.
  12. Nijhawan, R. (2002). Neural delays, visual motion and the flash-lag effect. Trends in cognitive sciences, 6(9), 387-393.
  13. Orban, G. A., Van Essen, D., & Vanduffel, W. (2004). Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humans. Trends in cognitive sciences, 8(7), 315-324.
  14. Palmer, S. E., & Kellman, P. J. (1991). A review of the “moving room” paradigm in studies of visual perception of self-motion. Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments, 1(3), 295-311.
  15. Saffell, T., & Cosgrove, D. (2010). A guide to analogue and digital motion capture systems. Computer animation and virtual worlds, 21(3-4), 259-268.
  16. Shadlen, M. N., & Newsome, W. T. (1998). The variable discharge of cortical

Top of Form

 

Sunday, April 16, 2023

Cognitive Psychology: Colour Perception and Cognitive Processes

 

 (CP-13) Colour Perception and Cognitive Processes



Abstract: Colour perception is an essential part of cognitive psychology, helping us understand how we interpret and make sense of the visual world. This article provides an overview of color perception for BS Cognitive Psychology students. It discusses the anatomy of the eye, including the retina, photoreceptor cells, rods, and cones, which are responsible for color vision. The human eye has three types of cones that are sensitive to different ranges of wavelengths, namely the red, green, and blue cones. The article explains how the brain processes the signals from these cones to create the perception of color, and how color constancy allows our brain to perceive colors accurately despite changes in the lighting environment. Additionally, the article covers color blindness, where individuals have difficulty distinguishing between certain colors. The article also examines four theories of color vision: the Trichromatic Theory, the Opponent-Process Theory, the Retinex Theory, and the Color Center Theory. These theories offer valuable insights into how we perceive color and how the brain processes visual information.

Introduction:  Top of Form

As humans, we see the world in a spectrum of colors, but have you ever wondered how we perceive colors? The answer lies in the study of color perception. Color perception is an essential part of cognitive psychology, as it helps us understand how we interpret and make sense of the visual world. In this article, we will delve into the basics of color perception and explore its various aspects.

Color Perception:

Color perception refers to the process by which our brain interprets the colors we see through our eyes. It involves the interaction of various parts of the visual system, including the retina, optic nerve, and brain. Our perception of color is not solely determined by the wavelength of light that enters our eyes, but also by the context and environment in which we view the colors.

The Anatomy of the Eye

Before we dive into the specifics of color perception, it's important to understand the anatomy of the eye. The eye is composed of various parts, including the retina, which is the layer of tissue at the back of the eye that contains photoreceptor cells. There are two types of photoreceptor cells in the retina, namely rods and cones. Rods are responsible for vision in low-light conditions, while cones are responsible for color vision in bright light.

Color Vision and the Three Types of Cones

The human eye has three types of cones that are responsible for color vision, namely the red, green, and blue cones. Each cone type is sensitive to a particular range of wavelengths of light. The brain processes the signals from these cones to create the perception of color. For instance, when all three types of cones are stimulated equally, we perceive the color white. On the other hand, when none of the cones are stimulated, we perceive the color black.

Color Constancy

Have you ever noticed that colors appear different under different lighting conditions? For instance, a white shirt may appear yellowish under incandescent light, but appear white under natural daylight. This phenomenon is known as color constancy, which is the ability of our brain to perceive colors as being constant despite changes in the lighting environment. This is achieved through a process called color normalization, which allows our brain to adjust for variations in lighting and still perceive colors accurately.

Color Blindness

Not everyone has normal color vision. Some individuals have a color vision deficiency, also known as color blindness. This condition affects the ability to perceive certain colors, and can be caused by genetic factors or certain medical conditions. The most common form of color blindness is red-green color blindness, where individuals have difficulty distinguishing between red and green colors.

Theories of color vision:

Trichromatic Theory: This theory, proposed by Thomas Young and Hermann von Helmholtz in the 19th century, suggests that there are three types of color receptors (cones) in the retina that are responsible for color vision. These cones are sensitive to different ranges of wavelengths, with one being most sensitive to short wavelengths (blue), one to medium wavelengths (green), and one to long wavelengths (red). The brain processes the signals from these cones to create the perception of color.

Opponent-Process Theory: This theory, proposed by Ewald Hering in the 19th century, suggests that color vision is the result of three opponent processes: red-green, blue-yellow, and black-white. According to this theory, cells in the retina and brain respond to pairs of opposing colors (e.g., red vs. green), and the perception of color is based on the relative activation of these opposing processes.

Retinex Theory: This theory, proposed by Edwin Land in the 1970s, suggests that color perception is based on the interaction between the color signals from the retina and the context in which they are seen. According to this theory, the brain compares the color of an object to the colors of its surrounding environment, and adjusts the perception of color accordingly.

Color Center Theory: This theory, proposed by Lawrence Hunt in the 1950s, suggests that color perception is the result of specialized cells in the brain that respond to specific wavelengths of light. According to this theory, there are two color centers in the brain: one that responds to long wavelengths (red) and another that responds to short wavelengths (blue-green). The perception of other colors is thought to be the result of the interaction between these two color centers.

These are just a few of the many theories of color vision that have been proposed over the years. While there is still much debate about which theory is the most accurate, they all offer valuable insights into how we perceive color.

References:

  1. Brainard, D. H. (1998). Color appearance and color constancy. In P. G. Grossenbacher (Ed.), Perception and the physical world: Psychological and philosophical issues in perception (pp. 63-90). John Wiley & Sons.
  2. Brainard, D. H., & Wandell, B. A. (1992). Asymmetric color matching: How color appearance depends on the illuminant. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 9(9), 1433-1448.
  3. Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Kiper, D. C. (2003). Color vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 26(1), 181-206.
  4. Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Sharpe, L. T. (1999). Color vision: From genes to perception. Cambridge University Press.
  5. Goldstein, E. B. (2019). Sensation and perception. Cengage Learning.
  6. Jameson, D., & Hurvich, L. M. (1989). Some quantitative aspects of an opponent-colors theory. II. Chromatic responses and spectral saturation. Vision Research, 29(5), 727-747.
  7. Mollon, J. D. (1999). Color vision: A clinical perspective. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9(4), 431-435.
  8. Nascimento, S. M. C., & Foster, D. H. (2013). Color constancy. In S. K. Shevell (Ed.), The Science of Color (2nd ed., pp. 357-386). Elsevier.
  9. Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision science: Photons to phenomenology. MIT press.
  10. Regan, B. C., & Mollon, J. D. (1997). Color vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 20(1), 399-430.
  11. Shevell, S. K. (2017). The science of color. Elsevier.
  12. Werner, J. S., & Chalupa, L. M. (2016). The new visual neurosciences. MIT press.
  13. Webster, M. A. (2015). Visual adaptation. Annual Review of Vision Science, 1, 547-567.
  14. Witzel, C., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2018). Categorical perception of color: History, concepts, and methods. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 57, pp. 1-74). Academic Press.
  15. Zeki, S. (1993). A vision of the brain. Blackwell Scientific Publications.
  16. Zeki, S. (1999). Art and the brain. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(6-7), 76-96.
  17. Zeki, S. (2015). The anatomy of illusions. Neuron, 88(4), 644-654.
  18. Zhang, P., & Brainard, D. H. (2019). The relationship between color naming, unique hues, and color perception. Journal of Vision, 19(11), 11-11.

Top of Form

Top of Form

Top of Form

 

Cognitive Psychology: Depth Perception; Understanding the World in Three Dimensions

 

(CP-12) Depth Perception: Understanding the World in Three Dimensions



Abstract: Depth perception is the ability to perceive objects in three dimensions, which is essential for navigating the world, avoiding obstacles, and interacting with objects. It relies on a combination of visual cues that can be divided into monocular cues, which require only one eye, and binocular cues, which require both eyes. The importance of depth perception is highlighted by its role in social interactions and performing everyday tasks such as driving and walking down stairs. Different brain regions are responsible for processing different aspects of depth perception, and some people may have difficulties with it due to conditions such as amblyopia and strabismus. As technology has advanced, virtual reality has become increasingly popular, and developers must pay close attention to depth perception cues to create a convincing experience. Researchers continue to study depth perception to gain a deeper understanding of how we perceive the world and develop new technologies to enhance our experiences.

Introduction:

As you go about your day, you are constantly interacting with the world around you. You walk down the street, pick up objects, and avoid obstacles. All of these actions require an understanding of depth perception, the ability to perceive objects in three dimensions.

What is Depth Perception?

Depth perception is the ability to perceive the world in three dimensions. This means that we can perceive how far away objects are from us, how much space they take up, and their relative position to other objects. Depth perception is crucial to our ability to navigate the world, avoid obstacles, and interact with objects.

How Does Depth Perception Work?

Depth perception relies on a combination of visual cues that our brain uses to interpret the distance and position of objects. These cues can be divided into two categories: monocular cues and binocular cues.

Monocular Cues

Monocular cues are visual cues that can be perceived with only one eye. These cues include:

  1. Perspective: Objects that are farther away appear smaller than objects that are closer to us.
  2. Texture Gradient: Objects that are farther away appear less detailed and less distinct than objects that are closer to us.
  3. Interposition: Objects that are closer to us partially block our view of objects that are farther away.
  4. Shadows: Shadows can provide information about the position and distance of objects.
  5. Motion Parallax: As we move, objects that are closer to us appear to move faster than objects that are farther away.

Binocular Cues

Binocular cues are visual cues that require both eyes to perceive. These cues include:

  1. Convergence: When we look at objects that are close to us, our eyes must turn inward (toward each other) to focus on the object.
  2. Retinal Disparity: Because our eyes are slightly separated, each eye receives a slightly different image of the world. Our brain uses these differences to create a sense of depth.
  3. Stereopsis: Stereopsis is the ability to use the slight differences between the images received by each eye to create a three-dimensional image of the world.

The Importance of Depth Perception:

Depth perception is crucial to our ability to navigate the world and interact with objects. Without depth perception, we would be unable to judge distances accurately, which could make it difficult to drive, walk down stairs, or perform many other everyday tasks.

Depth perception is also important for social interactions. It allows us to read other people's body language and facial expressions, which can help us understand their emotions and intentions.

Brain regions for depth processing: While we often take depth perception for granted, it is a complex process that involves multiple areas of the brain working together. Research has shown that different brain regions are responsible for processing different aspects of depth perception.

For example, the parietal cortex is involved in processing visual information about object position and motion. The occipital cortex is responsible for processing visual information about object shape and texture, while the temporal cortex is involved in processing visual information about object identity.

Disorders of Depth Perception:

Some people may have difficulties with depth perception. For example, people with amblyopia (also known as "lazy eye") may have impaired depth perception. In addition, people with strabismus (a condition where the eyes do not align properly) may also have difficulties with depth perception.

Depth Perception and Virtual Reality

As technology has advanced, virtual reality (VR) has become increasingly popular. VR is a simulated experience that allows users to interact with a three-dimensional environment. To create a convincing VR experience, developers must pay close attention to depth perception cues. For example, VR developers may use stereoscopic displays to create the illusion of depth, or use parallax effects to create the sensation of movement. In addition, they may use haptic feedback (vibrations or other tactile sensations) to further immerse users in the virtual environment.

Future Research on Depth Perception

As our understanding of depth perception continues to grow, researchers are exploring new ways to study this complex process. For example, some researchers are investigating the role of neural networks in depth perception, while others are studying how the brain processes information from different sensory modalities (such as sight and touch) to create a sense of depth.

Conclusion

Depth perception is a crucial aspect of our perception of the world around us. It allows us to navigate our environment and interact with objects, and is important for social interactions as well. Understanding the visual cues that contribute to depth perception can help us better understand how we perceive the world. Depth perception is an essential part of our perception of the world around us, allowing us to navigate our environment and interact with objects. While we often take depth perception for granted, it is a complex process that involves multiple areas of the brain working together. By continuing to study depth perception, we can gain a deeper understanding of how we perceive the world and develop new technologies to enhance our experiences.

References:

  1. Adams, W. J. (2012). A common signal processing framework for luminance and texture-based cues to depth. Perception, 41(8), 953-976.
  2. Bach-y-Rita, P., & Kercel, S. W. (2003). Sensory substitution and the human-machine interface. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(12), 541-546.
  3. Banks, M. S. (1988). The development of visual accommodation during early infancy. Child development, 59(6), 1464-1475.
  4. Baxter, L. A., & MacLeod, D. I. (1982). Magnitude estimation of slant from optic flow. Perception & Psychophysics, 31(5), 431-436.
  5. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial vision, 10(4), 433-436.
  6. Epstein, W., & Hatfield, G. (1990). Visual space perception: A primer. Blackwell Publishers.
  7. Foley, J. M., & Matlin, M. W. (2009). Sensation and perception. Wiley.
  8. Gajewski, D. A., Wallin, C. P., & Philbeck, J. W. (2014). Spatial updating relies on an egocentric representation of space: Effects of the number of objects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(3), 746-761.
  9. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin.
  10. Glennerster, A., & Tcheang, L. (2014). Cue combination in depth perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(5), 559-572.
  11. Goldstein, E. B. (2018). Sensation and perception (10th ed.). Cengage Learning.
  12. Howard, I. P., & Rogers, B. J. (2012). Perceiving in depth: Volume 1 basic mechanisms. Oxford University Press.
  13. Howard, I. P. (2012). Perceiving in depth: Volume 2 stereoscopic vision. Oxford University Press.
  14. Klein, R. (2014). Inattentional blindness. Wiley Online Library.
  15. Kellman, P. J., & Arterberry, M. E. (Eds.). (2006). The cradle of knowledge: Development of perception in infancy. MIT Press.
  16. Knill, D. C. (2012). Visual perception and brain function. In Oxford handbook of perception (pp. 385-414). Oxford University Press.
  17. Knill, D. C., & Richards, W. (Eds.). (1996). Perception as Bayesian inference. Cambridge University Press.
  18. Kosslyn, S. M., & Rosenberg, R. S. (Eds.). (2014). Series in affective science: Series editor Richard J. Davidson. Oxford University Press.
  19. Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Fujita, N., & Fukusima, S. S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 906-921.
  20. Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user's guide (2nd ed.). Psychology Press

Top of Form

 

Cognitive Psychology: Spatial vs. Linear Representation: Understanding the Differences

 

(CP-11) Spatial vs. Linear Representation: Understanding the Differences


Abstract: This article discusses the differences between spatial and linear representation in cognitive psychology, and their importance in various cognitive tasks and the development of cognitive skills in children. Spatial representation refers to the mental representation of space, including size, shape, and position, while linear representation is the mental representation of information in a linear, sequential manner. Spatial representation relies on mental imagery and perception, while linear representation is closely tied to language and memory. Examples of spatial representation include mental maps, object recognition, and navigation, while examples of linear representation include to-do lists, timelines, and steps of a recipe. The article highlights the importance of spatial and linear representation in navigation, object recognition, memory, spatial reasoning skills, organization of information, language and literacy skills, and more. By understanding these concepts, individuals can gain a better understanding of how their mind processes and organizes information.

Introduction:

As a student of Psychology, you may have come across the concepts of spatial and linear representation. These terms are often used in the field of cognitive psychology to describe how we mentally represent information in our minds. In this blog, we will discuss the differences between spatial and linear representation, their characteristics, and examples to help you understand them better.

Spatial Representation:

Spatial representation is the mental representation of space, including its size, shape, and position. It allows us to navigate our surroundings, understand the relationships between objects, and remember locations. Spatial representation is often used when we create mental maps or images of our environment.

Characteristics of Spatial Representation

  1. Imagery: Spatial representation relies heavily on the use of mental imagery. When we mentally visualize a map, we create a mental image of the spatial relationships between objects.
  2. Perception: Spatial representation is closely tied to perception. Our perception of our surroundings helps us create mental maps of our environment.
  3. Memory: Spatial representation is also linked to memory. Our ability to remember locations and navigate our environment is a function of our spatial memory.

Examples of Spatial Representation

  1. Mental maps: When you navigate a new place, you create a mental map of the area in your mind.
  2. Object recognition: When we recognize objects, we often use spatial information to do so. For example, we may recognize a car by its shape and size.
  3. Navigation: Our ability to navigate through our environment is a function of our spatial representation.

Linear Representation:

Linear representation is the mental representation of information in a linear, sequential manner. It involves organizing information in a structured way, often in a linear order. Linear representation is used when we create mental lists or organize information in a chronological order.

Characteristics of Linear Representation

  1. Order: Linear representation relies on the order of information. Information is organized sequentially, often in a chronological order.
  2. Language: Linear representation is closely tied to language. Language allows us to organize information in a structured way.
  3. Memory: Linear representation is also linked to memory. Our ability to remember information in a structured way is a function of our linear memory.

Examples of Linear Representation

  1. To-do lists: When you create a to-do list, you are using linear representation to organize your tasks in a structured way.
  2. Timelines: When we create timelines, we organize information in a chronological order.
  3. Steps of a recipe: When we follow a recipe, we organize the steps in a linear, sequential manner.

Spatial vs. Linear Representation: What's the Difference?

The main difference between spatial and linear representation is the way information is organized. Spatial representation organizes information based on its position in space, while linear representation organizes information in a structured, sequential way. Spatial representation relies on mental imagery and perception, while linear representation is closely tied to language and memory.

Importance of Spatial and Linear Representation in Cognitive Psychology

Spatial and linear representation are important concepts in cognitive psychology because they help us understand how the mind processes and organizes information. These types of mental representation are used in various cognitive tasks, such as navigation, object recognition, and memory. Spatial and linear representation are also important in the development of cognitive skills in children.

Navigation: Spatial representation is important for tasks that require us to navigate through our environment, such as driving, walking, or exploring a new place.

Object recognition: Spatial representation is also plays a role in object recognition, as we often use spatial information to recognize objects.

Memory: Spatial memory is important for remembering locations, directions, and spatial relationships between objects.

Spatial reasoning skills: Spatial representation is linked to the development of spatial reasoning skills, which are important for math and science.

Organization of information: Linear representation is important for tasks that require us to organize information in a structured way, such as creating lists, timelines, or following instructions.

Memory: Linear representation is also plays a role in memory, as we often remember information in a structured, sequential way.

Language and literacy skills: Linear representation is linked to the development of language and literacy skills, which are important for reading and writing.

Conclusion: Spatial and linear representation are two important concepts in cognitive psychology that help us understand how the mind processes and organizes information. These types of mental representation are used in various cognitive tasks, and are also important for the development of cognitive skills in children. Spatial representation allows us to mentally represent space, while linear representation allows us to organize information in a structured, sequential way. By understanding the differences between these two types of mental representation, you can gain a better understanding of how your mind processes and organizes information.

References:

  1. Bruineberg, J., Kiverstein, J., & Rietveld, E. (2018). The anticipating brain is not a scientist: The free-energy principle from an ecological-enactive perspective. Synthese, 195(6), 2417-2444.
  2. Cavanagh, P., & Alvarez, G. A. (2005). Tracking multiple targets with multifocal attention. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(7), 349-354.
  3. Chen, Z., & Klahr, D. (1999). All other things being equal: Acquisition and transfer of the control of variables strategy. Child development, 70(5), 1098-1120.
  4. Coren, S., Ward, L. M., & Enns, J. T. (2014). Sensation and perception. Wiley.
  5. Dehaene, S. (2014). Consciousness and the brain: Deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. Penguin.
  6. Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. The MIT Press.
  7. Hegarty, M., & Waller, D. (2005). A dissociation between mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abilities. Intelligence, 33(2), 175-191.
  8. Huttenlocher, J., & Lourenco, S. F. (2007). Sources of spatial cognition: behavioral and neural evidence. In Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 35, pp. 153-207). Elsevier.
  9. Jolicoeur, P., Gluck, M. A., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1984). Pictures and names: Making the connection. Cognitive psychology, 16(2), 243-275.
  10. Larson-Hall, J. (2010). A guide to doing statistics in second language research using SPSS. Routledge.
  11. Levine, S. C., Foley, A., Lourenco, S., Ehrlich, S., & Ratliff, K. R. (2016). Breaking down the gender divide: The importance of spatial skills for early mathematics learning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 231-240.
  12. Logie, R. H., & Gilhooly, K. J. (2018). Working memory and mental representation. Routledge.
  13. Mishra, J., & Mishra, R. (2019). Cognitive psychology: An overview. Research & Reviews: Journal of Educational Studies, 5(3), 8-12.
  14. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological review, 63(2), 81-97.
  15. Schönfeld, F., & Hoppe, U. (2019). Promoting spatial thinking in STEM fields. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 4(1), 35.
  16. Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (2019). Memory development between two and twenty. Psychology Press.
  17. Schwanenflugel, P. J., Stevens, K. T., & Kuo, L. J. (2016). The representation of linear order in language: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In The neural basis of reading (pp. 135-152). Springer.
  18. Shepard, R. N. (1984). Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological review, 91(4), 417-

Top of Form

 

Psychology: Forgetting; Nature, causes, theories and disorders

 

(ITP-16) Forgetting: Nature, causes, theories and disorders



Abstract: This article provides a comprehensive overview of forgetting, including its nature, causes, and related disorders. Forgetting is the inability to retrieve previously stored information from the memory, and it can happen due to various factors such as decay, interference, and retrieval failure.The article also explores various theories of forgetting, including decay theory, interference theory, and retrieval failure theory. Additionally, forgetting can be a symptom of disorders such as amnesia, Alzheimer's disease, and dementia. The article also provides techniques to improve memory retention, such as rehearsal, chunking, association, and visualization. Furthermore, factors such as age, stress, sleep deprivation, and medication can also influence forgetting. Finally, when forgetting becomes a problem, seeking professional help is important, as forgetting can be a symptom of depression, anxiety, and ADHD.Top of Form

Have you ever forgotten something important, like a friend's birthday or a meeting with your professor? Or perhaps you have struggled to recall an answer during a test, even though you studied extensively for it. These experiences are common and frustrating, but they are also part of a natural process known as forgetting.

What is forgetting?

Forgetting is the inability to retrieve information that was previously stored in the memory. This can happen for various reasons, including decay, interference, and retrieval failure. Decay refers to the gradual fading of memory traces over time. Interference happens when new information interferes with the retrieval of older information. Retrieval failure occurs when we are unable to access stored information due to inadequate cues or context.

Causes of Forgetting written in Theories:

Various theories have been proposed to explain the causes of forgetting. Here are some of the most prominent theories and their authors:

a.       Decay Theory by Ebbinghaus: This theory suggests that forgetting occurs due decay. Decay is the fading of memory traces over time. If we do not use or reinforce the memory traces, they will eventually be deleted.

b.      Interference Theory by Muller and Pilzecker: This theory suggests that forgetting occurs due to interference from other memories. Interference occurs when new information interferes with the retrieval of older information. Interference can happen in two ways:

·         Retroactive interference: new information interferes with the retrieval of old information.

·         Proactive interference: old information interferes with the retrieval of new information.

c.       Retrieval Failure Theory by Tulving and Thomson: This theory suggests that forgetting occurs due to Retrieval failure. Retrieval failure happens when we are unable to retrieve information due to inadequate cues or context. This can happen when we lack the necessary information or cues to access a particular memory.

d.      Motivated Forgetting Theory by Sigmund Freud: This theory suggests that forgetting occurs when we repress or suppress unpleasant or unwanted memories to protect ourselves from psychological harm.

e.      Encoding Failure Theory by Craik and Lockhart: This theory suggests that forgetting occurs due to inadequate encoding of information. If we do not encode information properly, it will not be stored in the memory.

Understanding these theories can help us better understand the nature of forgetting and develop strategies to improve memory retention.Top of Form

 

Disorders related to forgetting

Forgetting can also be a symptom of various disorders, such as:

  1. Amnesia: Amnesia is a condition where an individual is unable to remember past events or form new memories. It can be caused by head injuries, strokes, or degenerative diseases.

·         Retrograde amnesia is a type of amnesia where a person is unable to remember events that occurred before the onset of amnesia.

·         Anterograde amnesia, on the other hand, is a type of amnesia where a person is unable to form new memories after the onset of amnesia.

  1. Alzheimer's Disorder: Alzheimer's Disorder is a progressive brain disorder that affects memory, thinking, and behavior. It is the most common cause of dementia in older adults.
  2. Dementia: Dementia is a general term for a decline in cognitive function that interferes with daily life. It can be caused by various conditions, including Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and Huntington's disease.

How to improve memory retention

Although forgetting is a natural process, there are various techniques that can improve memory retention, such as:

  1. Rehearsal: Rehearsal involves repeating information to reinforce memory traces.
  2. Chunking: Chunking involves grouping information into smaller units to make it easier to remember.
  3. Association: Association involves linking new information to existing knowledge or memories.
  4. Visualization: Visualization involves creating mental images to aid memory retention.

Factors of Forgetting: Forgetting can be influenced by various other factors such as age, stress, sleep deprivation, and medication.

·         As we age, our memory and ability to retain new information may decline.

·         Stress and sleep deprivation can also affect memory, as they can interfere with the encoding and consolidation of new information in the memory.

·         Certain medications such as tranquilizers, sedatives, and antihistamines can also affect memory and lead to forgetting.

Forgetting is not always bad: It is important to note that forgetting is not always a bad thing. In fact, forgetting can be a useful process that helps us filter out irrelevant information and focus on what is important. Forgetting also allows us to update our knowledge and adapt to new situations.

Professional help for Forgetting: When forgetting becomes a problem and interferes with daily life, it is important to seek professional help. Forgetting can be a symptom of various disorders such as depression, anxiety, and ADHD. These conditions can affect memory and lead to forgetting, along with other symptoms such as difficulty concentrating, low mood, and anxiety.

Conclusion:

In summary, forgetting is a natural process that can occur due to various reasons. Memory is a complex system that can be influenced by various factors. Understanding the causes of forgetting and the techniques to improve memory retention can help in retaining important information. However, when forgetting becomes a problem and interferes with daily life, it is important to seek professional help.

References:

  1. Alzheimer's Association. (2021). Alzheimer's disease and dementia. Retrieved from https://www.alz.org/
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
  3. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 2, 89-195.
  4. Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417-423.
  5. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47-89.
  6. Craik, F. I., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671-684.
  7. Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Dover.
  8. Freud, S. (1899). Screen memories. The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, 3, 301-322.
  9. Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (2000). Principles of neural science (4th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  10. Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 13(5), 585-589.
  11. Muller, G. E., & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentelle beitrage zur lehre vom gedachtniss [Experimental contributions to the theory of memory]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 24, 1-300.
  12. Ranganath, C., & D'Esposito, M. (2001). Medial temporal lobe activity associated with active maintenance of novel information. Neuron, 31(5), 865-873.
  13. Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803-814.
  14. Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science, 253(5026), 1380-1386.
  15. Schacter, D. L. (2001). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 56(3), 205-218.
  16. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of Memory (pp. 381-403). New York: Academic Press.
  17. Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological review, 80(5), 352-373.

Top of Form

Top of Form

Top of Form

 

Psychology: Obedience and Helping behavior

(ITP-32) Obedience, Helping behavior Exploring Human Nature: Obedience, Helping Behavior, and Altruism Introduction: As psychology stud...